Skip to main content

Verified by Psychology Today

Bias

Why Do We Look Down on People Who Look Different?

Sources for the “anomalous is bad” facial stereotype.

Key points

  • People with facial anomalies, such as scars and birthmarks, are assumed to bear negative personal characteristics.
  • This “anomalous-is-bad” facial bias could arise from an evolved pathogen avoidance mechanism or it could be learned by culture.
  • We tested these alternate hypotheses in the hunter-gatherer Hadza and found the bias only emerged in those with exposure to non-Hadza culture.
  • The “anomalous-is-bad” bias appears to be learned culturally and can presumably be unlearned.
Photo by Alexander Voronov on Unsplash
Source: Photo by Alexander Voronov on Unsplash

Co-authored with Clifford I. Workman, PhD

Faces confer social benefits and costs. People imbue attractive faces with desirable characteristics, like being more trustworthy.1 These inferences have real-world consequences. For instance, people are more willing to borrow money from lenders with trustworthy faces.2 Dominance, judged from the faces of West Point cadets, has been linked to the promotions those military personnel would receive decades later.3 Competence, judged from the faces of people running for political office, has been associated with actual vote shares in elections,4 and political candidates facing conservative electorates benefit from looking more “stereotypically Republican” than their rivals.5 Especially troubling, the perceived trustworthiness of prisoners’ faces has been associated with whether they were given life sentences or death sentences for first-degree murder.6

Taken together, the “right” kinds of facial features can make one appear more trustworthy, can facilitate the pursuit of their career goals, and can even bear on life-or-death situations. If having the “right” kinds of facial features is a good thing, what does this mean for people with the “wrong” kinds of facial features—those that deviate from the norm?

The “Anomalous-is-Bad” Stereotype

We have reported that people whose faces have anomalous features—like scars, cleft lip and palate, or paralysis—suffer social penalties for looking different. People with anomalous faces are seen as less attractive and less trustworthy than people whose faces lack such features. They provoke implicit and explicit biases and even mistreatment by some viewers, and they trigger brain responses linked to those biases and behaviors.7-9 We call this suite of negative beliefs, biases, brain states, and behaviors the “anomalous-is-bad” stereotype.

Possible Sources of the Bias

Where do these negative associations with facial anomalies come from? One possibility is that we avoid interacting with people whose faces have anomalous features because those features signal the presence of a contagious disease. Since not all facial anomalies are signs of contagion, the anomalous-is-bad stereotype could be the overgeneralization of a mechanism that evolved to keep humans healthy.10

If the anomalous-is-bad stereotype is an evolutionary byproduct, then is it only human to look down on people who look different? Not necessarily. Even though most people don’t often interact with individuals whose faces are visibly different, negative cultural messages about facial anomalies could instead be the source of the anomalous-is-bad stereotype.

Using facial anomalies to signify moral corruption remains a popular device in Western storytelling.12 Anakin Skywalker’s physical and moral transformation into Darth Vader in the Star Wars prequels, the pockmarked face of the latest James Bond villain, and the Joker’s menacing smile that is quite literally carved into his face represent only a handful of examples. In the movie The Lion King, the baddest bad guy is even named “Scar.” Although the average person probably doesn’t have much firsthand experience with facial anomalies, negative tropes about anomalies permeate Western culture.

Both potential routes to the anomalous-is-bad stereotype—as a byproduct of an adaptation for avoiding pathogens, or as a byproduct of cultural learning—are plausible. How can we tell which one is right? This isn’t just a problem for researchers interested in facial differences—an overreliance on research volunteers from populations that are Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) threatens the generalizability of research in psychology and allied disciplines more generally.13 Even the face photographs researchers use in their studies are often overly-WEIRD.

If the anomalous-is-bad stereotype isn’t just a byproduct of social learning—if it’s linked to avoiding pathogens then we should find evidence for it in people who aren’t WEIRD. We teamed up with cross-cultural psychologists Kristopher Smith and Coren Apicella to test this idea in a recently published study.14 We looked for evidence of the anomalous-is-bad stereotype in members of a hunter-gatherer tribe in northwestern Tanzania, the Hadza.

The Hadza Hunter-gatherers

The Hadza hunter-gatherers’ lives are very different from that of the average person in a materially developed culture. Around 300 Hadza subsist on what they are able to hunt and forage. They typically live with 20-30 other Hadza in camps that move location every six to eight weeks as local resources are depleted. The Hadza vary in their exposure to non-Hadza culture, with some having had little to no exposure to the world outside of Hadza land. If the anomalous-is-bad stereotype is a tool for avoiding pathogens, we reasoned, it should be detectable in the Hadza—even in those Hadza with limited exposure to non-Hadza culture.

Using computer algorithms, we created photographs of never-before-seen Hadza and digitally superimposed scars onto their faces. We took these photographs to ten Hadza camps and asked Hadza volunteers about their reactions to the faces in the photographs. We showed them two face photographs—one with a scar and one without—and asked which person they expected to have better moral character.

At first, our results looked consistent with the idea that pathogen avoidance is at the root of the anomalous-is-bad stereotype. When asked who they thought had better moral character, the Hadza picked typical faces more often than scarred ones. When we took into account how much exposure each Hadza had to non-Hadza culture, though, evidence for the generalizability of the anomalous-is-bad stereotype vanished. In contrast to those Hadza with the most exposure to non-Hadza culture, those with the least exposure were equally likely to choose scarred faces as they were to choose typical ones when judging moral character. Since only Hadza with exposure to non-Hadza culture showed evidence of the anomalous-is-bad stereotype, our study suggests that this bias is learned.

Support for the cultural origin of the insidious anomalous-face-is-bad stereotype begets optimism: if the stereotype is learned, then it can also be unlearned.

References

Dion, K., Berscheid, E. & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285–290.

Suzuki, A. & Suga, S. (2010). Enhanced memory for the wolf in sheep’s clothing. Cognition, 117, 224–229.

Mueller, U. & Mazur, A. (1996). Facial dominance of West Point cadets as a predictor of later military rank. Social Forces, 74, 823–850.

Olivola, C. Y. & Todorov, A. (2010). Elected in 100 milliseconds: Appearance-based trait inferences and voting. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34, 83–110.

Olivola, C. Y., Sussman, A. B., Tsetsos, K., Kang, O. E. & Todorov, A. (2012). Republicans prefer Republican-looking Leaders. Social Psychology & Personality Science, 3, 605–613.

Wilson, J. P. & Rule, N. O. (2015). Facial trustworthiness predicts extreme criminal-sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 26, 1325–1331.

Jamrozik, A. et al. (2019). More than skin deep: Judgments of individuals with facial disfigurement. Psychology, Aesthetics, and the Creative Arts, 13, 117–129.

Workman, C. I. et al. (2021). Morality is in the eye of the beholder: the neurocognitive basis of the “anomalous‐is‐bad” stereotype. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1494, 3–17.

Hartung, F. et al. (2019). Behavioural and neural responses to facial disfigurement. Scientific Reports, 9, 8021.

Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R. & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function and structure. Psychological Review, 120, 65–84.

Ryan, S., Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J. & Case, T. I. (2012). Facial disfigurement is treated like an infectious disease. Evolution Human Behavior, 33, 639–646.

Croley, J. A., Reese, V. & Wagner, R. F. (2017). Dermatologic features of classic movie villains. JAMA Dermatology, 153, 559.

Apicella, C., Norenzayan, A. & Henrich, J. (2020). Beyond WEIRD: A review of the last decade and a look ahead to the global laboratory of the future. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41, 319–329.

Workman, C. I., Smith, K. M., Apicella, C. L. & Chatterjee, A. (2022). Evidence against the “anomalous-is-bad” stereotype in Hadza hunter gatherers. Scientific Reports, 12, 8693.

Bilici, N., Workman, C. I., Humphries, S., Hamilton, R. & Chatterjee, A. (2022). Normalizing Anomalies with Mobile Exposure (NAME): Reducing implicit biases against people with facial anomalies. in PsyArXiv Preprints.

advertisement
More from Anjan Chatterjee MD, FAAN
More from Psychology Today