Skip to main content

Verified by Psychology Today

Sleep

Sleep and the Social Brain

The sleeping brain uniquely functions to process social information.

While human culture and technologies have undoubtedly contributed to the need for greater resources being funneled into brain development among humans, it also appears that large brains and prolonged development are required to succeed in complex societies. Dunbar (1998) has shown that the association between neocortical volume and social group size among primates is very large (Pearson r=0.76). He (and many other distinguished neuroscientists) has articulated the “social brain hypothesis” which states that we have evolved big brains because of the social demands we face each day. Primates have larger than expected neocortical volumes (given body size) because they need to deal with the complexities of group or social life. Given neocortical volume in humans Dunbar theorized that the average human social network should be about 150 individuals. Ethnographic and social evidence supports this prediction. For example, it is the typical size of a hunter-gatherer group; of a company in a military organization, of a personal network (number of individuals a person knows directly); of a church congregation; of a small business company and so on. Within this large personal network there are hierarchically organized subgroups; alliances, coalitions and cliques etc that reflect differing degrees of familiarity with the individual at the center of the network.

Consider the cognitive demands such a hierarchically organized network places on individuals. Human societies succeed to the extent that problems can be solved cooperatively. Cooperation is based on trust and trust can only be earned over time and after many social interactions have taken place between the parties involved. Those social interactions have to be remembered, archived and recalled repeatedly when evaluating current trustworthiness of one’s potential partner in an enterprise. Maintaining the stability of relationships over time requires constant renegotiation of the terms of the cooperative agreement. People are capable of deception and thus that capacity needs to be taken into account when weighing evidence of trustworthiness. It requires that individuals learn how to read the intentions or minds of others in the group so as to manage conflict; anticipate strategic moves of the other and repair strained relationships and so on.

The capacity to appreciate that another individual has a mind like one’s own, capable of cooperation but also deception etc is called the “theory of mind” or ToM capacity. Dunbar points out that this capacity involves several layers of cognitive complexity. First order ToM involves having knowledge of one’s own mental states (“I believe that…”). Second order intentionality or ToM involves knowledge of another person’s mental states (“I believe that you understand that…”). Third order intentionality involves individual A thinking about what individual B is thinking about A’s thinking (“I intend that you think that I think that we are going to…”) etc. Most scholars working in the area of social cognition think that human beings are capable of perhaps 4-5 orders of intentionality but no more. The computational demands on the brain for this kind of social cognition must be considerable.

The areas of the brain that handle these computational demands around social cognition have been identified as the “social brain network”. The amygdala is important for evaluation of the emotions of self and others, particularly negative emotions. The fusiform gyrus supports rapid recognition and processing of faces. The face, of course, is crucial for social interactions as it emits all kinds of signals concerning the intentions and emotions of the individual. The ventromedial and dorsomedial prefrontal regions are known to support processing of Self-related information as well as understanding the mental states of others (i.e. ToM tasks). The frontopolar region (BA 10) evidences a uniquely complex structure in humans and is one of the evolutionarily most recent regions of the brain in primates. It is involved in multi-tasking, working memory and cognitive branching and therefore may support processing of 3rd and 4th etc orders of intentionality. The superior temporal sulcus contains mirror neurons that support social imitation behaviors and possibly emotional empathy. The temporal – parietal junction supports ToM tasks and language processing. The insula supports empathetic responses as well as moral emotions and the precuneus is involved in a range of activities from mental simulation to self-awareness. Finally the hippocampus is involved in memory functions. These are the major structures involved in social cognition but there are other regions as well that are sometimes included in the social brain and sometimes not—regions such as the ventral striatum and meso-cortical dopaminergic reward tracts and so on.

This “social brain” network handles all of the thinking and emotional work we have to do to keep track of and regulate our interactions with others. These interconnected brain structures allow us to fluently and more or less expertly, daily process huge amounts of strategic social information that is vital to our well-being and the well-being of those closest to us. It is no wonder that a network of interconnected structures specialize in handling all of this social information. What has all of this to do with sleep?

I propose that the sleeping brain uniquely involves this social brain network—or preferentially functions to maintain and repair this particular network. NREM takes the social brain off-line for repair and maintenance and then REM sleep reconnects its key structures and reactivate it in time for waking life. Sleep obviously facilitates repair and maintenance of other brain networks and regions but I am arguing that it is particularly important for functioning of the social brain.

Two decades of neuroimaging studies of the sleeping brain have suggested that roughly speaking the set of structures comprising the social brain are gradually taken off-line after sleep onset and throughout NREM sleep (basically the first half of the night) and then gradually put back together or re-connected and reactivated during each subsequent episode of REM until it fully comes back online after waking (see references list including Dang Vu et al. 2005; Maquet 2000; Maquet et al. 2005; Muzur et al. 2002).

Brain structures such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that help to regulate structures within the social brain are the first that are taken off-line during sleep onset and then the structures of the social brain itself are shut down in piecemeal fashion with each progressive episode of NREM (N1, N2 and N3 slow wave sleep). For example, in the first NREM episode of the night, there is relatively greater delta indexed slow wave activity (SWA) in frontal than in parietal and occipital regions (Werth et al. 1996, 1997; Finelli et al. 2001; Werth et al. 1996). Synchronization of slow wave activity then spreads progressively to posterior cortical zones. Meanwhile delta waves propagate up through subcortical nodes of the social brain network as NREM sleep progresses through the first half of the night.

Progressive deactivation of the brain within NREM sleep is centered on regions in the social brain network including the medial prefrontal cortex, anterior medial areas in BA 9 and 10, orbitofrontal cortices (BA 11) including caudal orbital basal forebrain, anterior cingulate (BA24), bilateral anterior insula, basal forebrain/anterior hypothalamus, bilateral putamen and left precuneus (Dang Vu et al. 2005; Kaufmann et al. 2006).

All of this de-activation of brain sites is reversed with the onset of REM during the second half of the night. REM involves the reconnection and activation of a huge swath of midline and limbic and paralimbic regions including especially the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and other key nodes of the social brain network (see review of REM neuroimaging studies in the Dang Vu and the Maquet et al references below).

If this speculation is correct then we have the remarkable fact that sleep involves the deactivation, dis-connection/dismantling of social brain structures during the NREM phases of the first half of the night and then the reactivation and reconnection of these structures during the REM phases in the latter half of the night. Presumably this dis- and re-assembly of connectivity between nodes in the social brain network serves the functional purpose of efficiently processing vitally important social information acquired during the day but left unprocessed or unevaluated. If this scenario is even partially correct then sleep is not only important for bodily repair, or brain repair or memory functions in general--it is vitally important for social cognition in particular.

References

References

Achermann P, Finelli LA, Borbely AA (2001) Unihemispheric enhancement of delta power in human frontal sleep EEG by prolonged wakefulness. Brain Research 913:220-223.

Braun, A.R., T.J. Balkin, N.J. Wesensten, R.E. Carson, M. Varga, P. Baldwin, S, Selbie, G. Belenky & Herscovitch, P. (1997) Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. Brain 120:1173-1197.

Braun, A.R., T.J. Balkin, N.J Wesensten, F. Gwadry, R.E. Carson, M. Varga, P. Baldwin, G. Belenky, & Herscovitch, P. (1998) Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye-movement sleep. Science 279:91-95.

Corsi-Cabrera M, Miro E, del-Rio-Portilla Y, Perez-Garci E, Villanueva Y, Guevara MA.Rapid eye movement sleep dreaming is characterized by uncoupled EEG activity between frontal and perceptual cortical regions. Brain Cogn. 2003 Apr;51(3):337-45.

Czisch M, Wehrle R, Kaufmann C, Wetter TC, Holsboer F, Pollmacher T, Auer DP. Functional MRI during sleep: BOLD signal decreases and their electrophysiological correlates. Eur J Neurosci. 2004 Jul;20(2):566-74.

Czisch M, Wetter TC, Kaufmann C, Pollmacher T, Holsboer F, Auer DP. Altered processing of acoustic stimuli during sleep: reduced auditory activation and visual deactivation detected by a combined fMRI/EEG study. Neuroimage. 2002 May;16(1):251-8.

Finelli, L.A., Borbely, A.A. & Achermann, P. (2001) Functional topography of the human nonREM sleep electroencephalogram. European Journal of Neuroscience 13:2282-2290.

Dang-Vu TT, Desseilles M, Albouy G, Darsaud A, Gais S, Rauchs S, Schabus M, Sterpenich S, Vandewalle G, Schwartz S and Maquet P. Neuroimaging of Dreaming; Swiss Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry (in press)

Dang-Vu TT, Desseilles M, Laureys S, Degueldre C, Perrin F, Phillips C, Maquet P, Peigneux P. Cerebral correlates of delta waves during non-REM sleep revisited. Neuroimage. 2005 Oct 15;28(1):14-21. Epub 2005 Jun 23.

De Gennaro L, Vecchio F, Ferrara M, Curcio G, Rossini PM, Babiloni C. Changes in fronto-posterior functional coupling at sleep onset in humans. J Sleep Res. 2004 Sep;13(3):209-17.

Dunbar, R. (1998). The Social Brain Hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology 6:178–190

Guevara MA, Lorenzo I, Arce C, Ramos J, Corsi-Cabrera M. Inter- and intrahemispheric EEG correlation during sleep and wakefulness. Sleep. 1995 May;18(4):257-65.

Hofle, N., Paus, T., Reutens, D., Fiset, P., Gotman, J., Evans, A.C. & Jones, B.E. Regional cerebral blood flow changes as a function of delta and spindle activity during slow wave sleep in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience 17, 4800-4808 (1997).

Hong, C.C.H., Gillin, J.C., Dow, B.M., Wu, J. & Buchsbaum, M.S. (1995) Localized and lateralized cerebral glucose metabolism associated with eye movments during REM sleep and wakefulness: A positron emission tomography (PET) study. Sleep 18:570-80.

Kaufmann C, Wehrle R, Wetter TC, Holsboer F, Auer DP, Pollmacher T, Czisch M. Brain activation and hypothalamic functional connectivity during human non-rapid eye movement sleep: an EEG/fMRI study. Brain. 2006 Mar;129(Pt 3):655-67. Epub 2005 Dec 9.

Madsen, P.C., Holm, S., Vorstup, S., Friberg, L., Lassen, N.A. & Wildschiodtz, L.F. (1991) Human regional cerebral blood flow during rapid eye movement sleep. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism 11:502-7.

Manford M, Andermann F. (1998) Complex visual hallucinations: Clinical and neurobiological insights. Brain 121:1819-1840.

Maquet, P. Functional neuroimaging of normal human sleep by positron emission tomography. Journal of Sleep Research 9, 207-231 (2000).

Maquet, P., Smith, C., Stickgold, R. eds. (2003) Sleep and Brain Plasticity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Maquet, P., Peters J.M., Aerts, J., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., & Franck, G. (1996) Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature 383:163-66.

Maquet, P., Degueldre, C., Delfiore, G., Aerts, J., Peters, J.M., Luxen, A., & Franck, G.(1997) Functional neuroanatomy of human slow wave sleep.The Journal of Neuroscience 17:2807-12.

Maquet P, Ruby P, Maudoux A, Albouy G, Sterpenich V, Dang-Vu T, Desseilles M, Boly M, Perrin F, Peigneux P, Laureys S. Human cognition during REM sleep and the activity profile within frontal and parietal cortices: a reappraisal of functional neuroimaging data. Prog Brain Res. 2005;150:219-27.

Maquet P, Ruby P, Schwartz S, Laureys S, Albouy G, Dang-Vu T, Desseilles M, Boly M, Peigneux P. Regional organisation of brain activity during paradoxical sleep (PS); Archives Italiennes de Biologie 142 (4), 2004, p. 413-9.

Muzur A, Pace-Schott EF, Hobson JA. (2002) The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16:475-481.

Nofzinger EA, Buysse DJ, Miewald JM, Meltzer CC, Price JC, Sembrat RC, Ombao H, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Hall M, Kupfer DJ, Moore RY. (2002) Human regional cerebral glucose metabolism during non-rapid eye movement sleep in relation to waking. Brain 125:1105-15.

Werth, E. Achermann P. & Borbely A.A. Brain topography of the human sleep EEG: antero-posterior shifts of spectral power. NeuroReport 8, 123-127 (1996).

Werth, E., Achermann P. & Borbely A.A. Fronto-occipital EEG power gradients in human sleep. Journal of Sleep Research 6, 102-112 (1997).

advertisement
More from Patrick McNamara Ph.D.
More from Psychology Today