Skip to main content

Verified by Psychology Today

Forecasting the Future

Why our inability to predict emotion may be beneficial. Every
decision is made based on the belief that it will ultimately make us
happier than would an alternative choice.

Every decision, big or small, is made based on the belief that it
will ultimately make us happier than would any alternative choice.
Unfortunately, human beings aren't very accurate in predicting their
emotional reactions to future events. Daniel Gilbert, Ph.D., a Harvard
University psychology professor, discusses his research on this tendency,
called affective forecasting, and why we don't really want to improve our
power to predict.

Susan Fiske: What is affective forecasting?

Daniel Gilbert: It's the study of how and how well people predict
their feelings should particular events unfold.

How does it play into our everyday lives?

We take action predicated upon the belief-explicit or implicit-that
one action will lead to greater rewards than some alternative action.
Whether we're trying to decide what to have for breakfast, or whether or
not to get married, every decision is based on the belief that one choice
will probably lead us to feel better than another choice.

How have you investigated this notion?

We've studied minute events, such as receiving negative personality
feedback, and events of greater importance, such as the election of
presidents. What we find in all of these instances is pretty much the
same: People almost always overestimate the intensity and duration of
their future emotional reactions. For instance, when asked to predict how
they will feel two months after a breakup, people often expect to feel
devastated. But when you compare them with those who have already gone
through a breakup, you find that the effects tend to be milder and wear
off much more quickly than was predicted.

Why are we so bad at forecasting our own emotions?

There are a number of reasons. One phenomenon we've studied is
called focalism. If, for instance, I were to ask you how you would feel
six months after your child died, you would probably get a mental image
that shakes you to your very foundation. Then you might imagine how you'd
feel in six months and think, "I suppose I'd feel a little less
devastated later." What you don't do is imagine any of the other events
that might happen in those six months-interacting with your remaining
children, for instance, or going to a party-that have a small but
cumulative impact on your recovery. A second reason is something we call
immune neglect. Human beings have the ability to make the best of a bad
situation. After anticipating a devastating divorce, say, people find
that their spouses were never really right for them. I like to say that
people have a psychological immune system. We suffer the slings and
arrows of outrageous fortune more capably than we might predict.

So just like we get over a cold faster than we might anticipate, we
also get over psychological assaults more quickly.

The difference between the physical and psychological immune
systems is that we know we have a physical immune system and that we'll
get over a cold pretty quickly. The psychological immune system tends to
be invisible to us, so we act as though we're out there without any
psychological defenses. But once these defenses spring into action, we
surprise ourselves with how resilient we really are.

Can we become more accurate in affective forecasting?

Probably, but first we should ask whether or not we want to. It's
very easy to see somebody making a logical error and say, "Well, you
ought not to have made it." But logical errors can serve an important
purpose in human cognition. Imagine a world in which some people realize
that external events have much less impact than others believe they do.
Those who make that realization might not be particularly motivated to
change the external events. But one of the reasons we protect our
children, for example, is that we believe we would be devastated if they
were harmed or killed. So these predictions may be very effective in
motivating us to do the things we as a society need to do, even though
they might be inaccurate on an individual level. Anyone who wanted to
cure affective forecasters of their inferential ills would be wise to
measure both the costs and benefits of forecasting errors.

What advice would you offer instead?

Grandmothers' advice: "It's always darkest before the dawn"; "There
are other fish in the sea"; "Time heals all wounds." However, when these
cliches are presented to people who are suffering, they may fall on
deaf ears, so I'm not sure that it does a lot of good to present them
with facts on how quickly people tend to recover from catastrophe, trauma
or loss.