Skip to main content

Verified by Psychology Today

Productivity

Status Hierarchies: Do We Need Them?

Are social hierarchies a natural part of human society?

The following is a post that also appeared on Psych Your Mind. I thought I'd share these thoughts about social status with Psychology Today! I'd love to read your responses here or on twitter @mwkraus!

I have been studying the topic of social status ever since I started my graduate training. That was in 2004 when George W. Bush was starting his second term as president, Clint Eastwood was busy winning an Oscar for best picture (Million Dollar Baby), and Lindsay Lohan wasn’t a punchline. In all of that time, I hadn’t ever considered the question of whether society needs social hierarchies in the first place? That is, do we really need to rank ourselves in society relative to others? Is it necessary to have varying levels of power, prestige, and status in society? Or could society function quite well without differentiation based on status?

Clearly there are some good anecdotes that support the notion that hierarchy is unnecessary. For instance, there is an excellent pizza joint in Berkeley called Cheeseboard. It’s actually the Cheese Board Collective, which is owned and operated by a cooperative group of individuals who each share in the work and the profits of the business. There are no explicit status hierarchies at the Cheeseboard, and they make some pretty excellent pizza!

Unfortunately, there aren’t too many other examples of groups or societies without social hierarchy. Which made me wonder: Why is that?

It’s important to point out the adaptive benefits of having hierarchies in society. For instance, Emile Durkheim (pictured in the teaser) argued in the Division of Labor (or Labour) in Society (1893) that people differ in ability, and that these abilities lead us to take on a different role in the labor force. Thus, some people will be turning screws and some people will be managing others. Durkheim, for his part, seemed to think that this division was going to be awesome for everyone because all people could be part of a larger whole—the manager and the screw-turner both contribute in their own unique way toward making an automobile. However, one of the consequences of a division of labor is that some people are going to have better jobs than others, more freedom and autonomy in their work, and also, more respect, prestige, and status.

So, hierarchies seem to form naturally out of divisions of labor in society. That’s fine, but maybe people don’t notice these status differences? Maybe, the screw- turner and the manager don’t see their jobs as that different, and don’t think of one job as higher status than the other? It’s possible, but not likely that people ignore status differences in society. Why might that be the case?

Social groups can gain benefits from having divisions of labor—they increase productivity by allowing people to specialize their skills (Durkheim, 1893). Groups can also gain benefits from signaling and perceiving status differences. The reasoning behind this point is simple: If I have a problem that I need help with, or resources to solve, I need to be able to quickly and accurately discern which people, among my social group, have the necessary resources and skills to help me with my problem. By this logic, humans should have been developing a clear signaling process for status differences in social groups for centuries.

There is some mounting evidence suggesting that the nonverbal display of pride evolved to serve this status signaling function. Pride is an emotion experienced when one achieves socially valued success. The typical pride display includes expanding the chest, clenching the fists, placing the arms on the hips or raising the arms in a “V” shape, and showing a small and controlled smile.

If the pride expression evolved to serve a status signaling function in social groups. If this is true, then pride displays should be associated with status cross-culturally. Recent research by Jess Tracy at the University of British Columbia and her colleagues suggests that this might be true.

In a 2008 study conducted by Tracy along with David Matsumoto of San Francisco State University, the emotional expressions of Olympic judokas were examined following wins or losses at the 2004 Athens Olympics. They found that the prototypical pride display was shown following wins (vs. losses) cross-culturally. This means that if whether you are from Malaysia or Miami, you tend to celebrate a victory in the same fashion, using the same pride display. That’s cool, but that wasn’t the best part of the study! In a second sample, Tracy and Matsumoto examined pride expressions for congenitally blind judokas at the 2004 Paralympic games. They found the same pattern of nonverbal pride expressions following wins (vs. losses) for these blind athletes. These results suggest that pride evolved for the communication of one’s socially valued position (Tracy & Matsumoto, 2008). That congenitally blind athletes showed the same pride expressions as their sighted counterparts is indicative of the fact that pride displays are not simply expressions we mimic based on viewing our peers experiencing success. Pride expressions seem to have a richer history in human ancestry, and may be a universal expression of emotion.

This evidence seems to suggest that, while it seems nice to think of a context where we could all be free from status differences, it’s likely tough to achieve that sort of utopia. For instance, I wonder what happens at the Cheeseboard when one person in the collective is complimented for their work and others are not? Status hierarchies might form even in that context, explicitly designed to minimize status differences.

What do you think about status hierarchies? Should/could we rid ourselves of status in society? I’d love to read your comments!

References

Tracy JL, & Matsumoto D (2008). The spontaneous expression of pride and shame: evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105 (33), 11655-60 PMID: 18695237.

advertisement
More from Michael W Kraus Ph.D.
More from Psychology Today